Managing anxiety and task through self-efficacy: an assessment of creative people How can creative people improve on task and anxiety management through self-efficacy?
https://ilokabenneth.blogspot.com/2019/07/managing-anxiety-and-task-through-self.html
Author: Iloka Benneth Chiemelie
Published: 30th July 2019
Published: 30th July 2019
Introduction
Self-efficacy, which is all about having trust on one’s
powers and ability to learn and perform, is considered a pivotal trait when it
comes to general success of people (Hill, 2002).
Whether it is in the academic setting or in career field, when one can
understand their weakness, and believe within themselves that they can conquer
such, they become better positioned to change their lives positively. As
described by Gardner (1983), students that
are self-efficacious, are those that believe in their capabilities to organize
and execute the necessary actions that would help them to attain a given goal. McCombs & Marzano (1990) and Martinez-Pons (2002) further moved on to classify
self-efficacy into different categories and one of them is the academic
category, where it was noted by these authors that it reflects on the perceived
capabilities that students have when it comes to a task that they are expected
to perform within the academic domain.
A definition of self-efficacy was offered by Ollendick, Dailey, & Shapiro (1983), where it
was pointed out to be the process used to activate and sustain thoughts,
emotions and behaviours in line with set goals. In the case that these goals entail
learning, the self-regulation of these individuals are then converted into
self-regulated learning. On that note, the self-regulated learners are then
capable of combining their academic learning skills with that of their
self-control in order to make their learning process easier; and the implication becomes that
these people are more motivated, leading to acquisition of more skills and the
desire to learn more (Murphy & Alexander, 2000).
This is also the case for creative people, where their cognitive abilities and
self-efficacy has been recognized in literatures and sufficiently explored (Schunk, 2004). Delineating on the relationship
between creativity and cognitive self-efficacy might necessitate the need to
study the cognitive approaches, relevant intelligence theories, regulation of
resources, and attainment of goals.
In essence, the creative people might be able to employ
meta-cognition, which can be considered as “thinking about thinking “, or the
“knowledge about knowing and learning”, a reference to a higher degree of
cognition that is employed for monitoring and regulating the cognitive
processes like comprehension, reasoning, learning, problem-solving and others (Metcalfe & Shimamura, 1994). The implication
is that such people can effectively handle their resources, have a higher
believe on their pliable intelligence, work towards mastery instead of just
performance and as such capable of bringing about better performance (Dweck, 2006). Essentially, when such people are
faced with anxiety or task related challenges, they are better capable of
handling them through self-efficacy and this is what this research aims to
assess. In any case, it has been noted that since people differ in their
metacognitive skills and knowledge, they also differ in relation to how well as
fast they learn (Brown, Bransford, Ferrara, &
Campione, 1983; Morris, 1990). This is in line with the postulations of
the social cognitive theory, where it is maintained that while some learn
through observation, others actually learn through cognitive skills (Bandura, 2001).
Research objectives
Based on the discussions above, the overall objective of
this research is to discuss on how creative people can employ self-efficacy to
manage anxiety and task. That is to say, how can task management and anxiety
management be made easier by creative people?
Research outline
In order to meet this set objective, this research will
explore the concepts of anxiety management and task management as the dependent
variables, as well as the concept of self-efficacy as the independent variable,
and then highlight how the independent variable can be effectively employed to
bringing about positive influence on the dependent variables. This study is
literature.
Understanding anxiety
and its relationship with creative thinking
Across the world, anxiety is considered to be the most
common mental disorder that affects millions of adolescents. In the course of
adolescence, it is considered to be one of the most devastating consequences
that comes with psychiatric disorder (Fiori et al.,
2010). In line with the work of Khouzam
(2009), anxiety disorders are among the most frequently experienced
emotional, mental, and behavioural issues that people face in numerous
countries. The concept of anxiety is
mostly featured in the form of unpleasant, diffused, complex form of
apprehension, normally followed by autonomic symptoms like perspiration,
headache, tightness, palpitation, restlessness, and mild stomach discomfort,
showing on the side of the person suffering it, the inability to sit properly
or stall still for a long period of time (Sadock
and Sadock, 2007). In the course of the past years, the concept of
anxiety can been classified by researchers into different categories with Freud
proposing signal and traumatic anxiety while Sadeghi
et al. (2010) looked at anxiety from the view of generalized anxiety
disorder (GAD).
From a historical sphere, the 20th century was
referred as the “anxiety century” as a result of the incessant wars, increase
in population, immigration, unemployment, and destruction of families (Passer et al., 2009).
When it comes to assessing the influence of anxiety on
creative thinking, the theory of anxiety and creative thinking can be traced
back to the early days of humankind. From the work of Freud, the suggestion is
that anxiety is a kind of stress that is brought about by birth separation and
the experience that human have in relation to their biological fantasies. Rank (1993), Klein (1975) and Winnicott (1999)
later supported this theory in relation to creativity in human. Based on this,
it was suggested by Passer et al. (2009)
that creative thinking can be employed as a tool for reducing the negative
influence of anxiety on people as well as the overall level of anxiety
experienced. The importance of this is based on the notion that creative
thinking is a skill that is used to bring about the power of discovery and new
ideas as well as help in changing the negative feeling that people have to
positive feeling and in return bring about improvement on their mental health
in the case that they experience unpleasant event. In any case, it was later
revealed by Silvia and Kimbrel (2010) that
anxiety and depression does predict little about the differences in creativity
when the directions are inconsistent.
That is to say, the conjectures of creativity do vary on a number of
issues, but consensus has been attained by scholars with goes to say that there
is no way of stating how people become creative or the influence of anxiety on
creativity (Beghetto and Kaufman, 2007).
Carlson (2002) furthered
this study and came up with the conclusion that creativity does offer defence
mechanism for anxiety with this work being later supported by that of Henderson, Rosen and Mascaro (2007) where it was
demonstrated that drawing (a stage in creativity) does prove a calming effect
on patients suffering post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). From a related
sphere, it was also demonstrated by Mitchell,
MacDonald and Knussen (2008) that music is a therapeutic key that is
used to bring about significant decline on the level of anxiety that people
suffer. This is based on the notion that the creative power one obtains through
education in life in the course of growing up could form a mental safeguard
against disorders related to mental health. Potur
and Barkul (2010) furthered this discussion
with their suggestion that creativity is important when it comes to providing
solutions to the everyday problems of planning and decision making. Pivotal
life decisions like career planning, choosing employment spouses, and places of
residence, and making crucial commitments does require thinking that are
divergent in nature (creative thinking), one that is particularly relevant to
creativity. On its own, life experience involves situations that are
open-ended, having no clear visible external sources of correctness or truth
and ambiguous. Therefore, the perception is that personalities that are highly
anxious will face difficulties in these areas. In other studies, fear and anxiety have been
linked with decrease in the quality of life Potur
and Barkul (2010), with similar literatures stating that the ability of
individuals to show courage in the face of difficulties does bring about
increase in their overall well-being (Joseph &
Linley, 2005).
In essence, research outcomes in relation to the influence
of anxiety on creativity have been mixed with some researchers finding positive
correlation between anxiety trait and creativity (Carlsson,
Wendt, & Risberg, 2000; Martindale, Anderson, Moore, & West, 1996)
while others have noted that anxiety interferes with or is not related to
creative performance (Byron & Khazanchi, 2011;
De Dreu, Nijstad, & Baas, 2011; King, Walker, & Broyles, 1996).
Thus, this present study will look at this relationship in relation to how
self-efficacy can be used to control anxiety in order to enhance the
performance of creative people.
Task management and
its relationship with creativity
A topic that has been the frequent subject of scholarly
research is the ability of people to manage their time wisely and also the
extent to which they misappropriate time (Claessens
et al. 2007; Steel2007). In line with general view, time management
imply a strategic process that is used to promote the accomplishment of vital
goals and success with an individual’s professional, personal, and academic
setting (Claessens et al. 2007; Steel, 2007).
On the other hand, procrastination entails unnecessary delay and dilatory
behaviour that individuals exhibit which is normally considered to be the
misuse of time, one that will eventually limit their performance as well as
impede their ability to reach set goals (Chu and
Choi 2005; Lay and Schouwenburg 1993; Steel, 2010). Based on this
theoretical understanding, one might take the assumption that time management
and procrastination are discordant in nature and would essential bring about
clear negative relationship. However, there are limited number of empirical
studies that test this assumption (e.g., Chu and
Choi 2005; Lay and Schouwenburg 1993; Park and Sperling 2012). On the
same note, it has been argued by some researchers that under some
circumstances, deliberate delay in one’s work (take academic work for
instance), can actually be measured as an adaptive expression of time
management which would eventually lead to academic success for the person in
question (Choi and Moran 2009; Kim and Seo, 2013;
Schraw et al. 2007).
The modern Gregorian calendar and mechanical clock is
predated by time management. In line with the views presented above, both
scholars are laypeople have shared their own views on how best to manage time
for centuries above (e.g., Alberti, 1971; Seneca, 2014; St. Benedict, 1975) a
fact that confirms the perennial pervasiveness of time management. However,
there is no widely accepted definition of time management (Claessens et al., 2007). In the past, some have
considered it to be a combination of goal setting, time assessment, and
monitoring activities (while it has been viewed by others to be a
self-controlled attempt geared towards using time in a way that is subjectively
effective with the aim of attaining a desired outcome (Koch
& Kleinmann, 2002, p. 201), and others have actually disregarded the
need to define time management in its entirety (e.g.,
Barling, Cheung, & Kelloway, 1996).
The issues with defining time management are based on the
fact that there are slightly different takes on time management from different
disciplines. Taking the field of sociology for instance, when it comes to time
management, emphasis might be placed on personal time while in the field of
psychology, the emphasis might be more on the ability of an individual to stick
to plans and make precise estimation of how long such task will be undertaken.
As such, it is required that any definition of time management should be one
that subsumes, integrates, and is applicable to different disciplines. This
will require the adoption of a person-centred view in which individuals should
be conceptualized as proactive and intentional agents (Aguinis
& Glavas, inpress; Rupp, 2011). Based on this view, the contention
is that people make the decision about how they allocate time. As such, time
management can be defined to be a form of decision that is made by an
individual, one that is used for structuring, protecting, and adapting to time
with the overall aim of changing the person’s conditions. This definition is in
line with the agent perspective of time (Granqvist
& Gustafsson, 2016).
Time management is an integral part of the task management
process because it is only when the time allocated for each task has been
properly managed, that the actual process of executing and finalizing the tasks
can be effectively managed (Granqvist &
Gustafsson, 2016). In essence, creativity is integral here because it
would help the individual to effectively and efficiently manage their time,
yielding an effective and efficient task outcomes in the end.
From the managerial standpoint, creativity is considered to
be a subjective assessment (Amabile, 1982),
the creation of original and vital ideas (Amabile,
1983; Amabile, 1996; Arndt et al., 1999), a complex activity that
features creative thinking, expertise and motivation (Amabile,
1998), a complex interaction that an individual creates with one’s
environment (Anderson et al., 2014; Pruskus, 2015),
a societal activity in a given context (Ford and
Gioia, 2000), a critical process (Drazin et
al., 1999), a form of divergent thinking that include flexibility,
fluency, elaboration, and originality (Paulus, 2000)
and the list goes on. Aside from this, the history of human thinking does
portray an evolution of creative of creativity understanding (Barevičiūtė, 2014; Černevičiūtė, 2014), together
with different forms of understanding in different regions or even within the
same region that is culturally diverse (Klimczuk,
2014).
On this accord, it has been regarded by researchers that the
modern society is highly creative (Pečiulis, 2015;
Reimeris, 2016), which points out the need for task management to be
effectively studied and analyzed. There
seem to be a consensus among scholars, how that places more emphasis on
creativity management as being contradictable and paradoxical, but they still
agree that creative people are better positioned to manage their tasks.
This is based on the notion that when creative people have
different tasks, or different targets and levels of achievement for the same
task, they have the necessary instinct to yield their course of action towards
meeting the settings that have been defined in these tasks (McLeod et al., 1997; Amabile, 1998; Sutton, 2001; Bilton
and Cummings, 2014; Lane and Lup, 2015; Johnsen, 2015; Chen et al., 2015).
They can potentially combine different activities at the same time or looking
for more effective ways of doing things, leading to better fulfilment of the
task targets. In essence, creativity is essential when it comes to task
management because it helps people to do things more effectively and
efficiently.
Usually, the work time of a creative person is not normally
standardized. On the same note, it is virtually impossible to lock such person
up in one place in the course of a defined time because of the creative nature
of their work. On another hand, their work time normally violates the work
hours that have been defined by either their employer or law; because such
people are found of creating even when they are eating, sleeping or during
vacation.
Creativity management
and control
The tools employed in production are different, ranging from
papers and pencils to the more sophisticated and expensive ones – such as
antique kiln, computers and so on. However, all these tools are entirely based
on the fact that the major creation process is contained within the heads of
the creative workers, with these tools only serving as aid to the creative
worker to realize their ideas. This is contrary with what is obtainable in the
factor setting where thinking is left excluded from the worker. Companies
normally employ a contrary notion to this by excluding “thinking” from the
workers, one that forgets or at least neglects the fact that it is actually the
individual that is doing the thinking. This is because the contemporary tools
do the thinking instead of the workers. However, it needs to be pointed out
that no creative workers starts from nowhere, instead, they are products of
combination of ideas and pictures from the menu of ideas available with the
overall aim of attaining new harmony in their mix. In the end, they produce
creative products that are capable of doing unique and exclusive things. Such
products can have both positive and negative features as one product that might
be attractive to one consumer might be unsalable to another.
In accordance with the work of Florida
(2002), it was pointed out that all the administrative rules of the work
class, country class, and even the service class are not in line with the views
of the creative class and the implication is that the creative class requires
different forms of rules and control. In essence, the overall aim of such rule
is to force efficiency into a worker irrespective of how such is delivered,
which is, to ensure that the worker is able to produce as much products as
possible within the shortest time by economizing as many resources as possible
and saving these resources for production. The overall aim of control in this
case does become to check a worker, and understand the extent to which such worker
is adhering to the conditions of the work contract, i.e., if the worker is
gives all of his or her time and potential towards delivering the desired
objective from their designated work and ensuring that the company or employer
is getting the best of return from their contacted salary. The presupposition
of control in this case is that of the employer’s belief, holding that a worker
in a workplace doesn’t have one’s body, which is only part of the mechanism
that is employed in a company. All the employees that are making use of the
social resources are controlled by the state. The presupposition in this case
then becomes that every taxpayer in a given system does serve the social
purpose of producing social welfare and happiness, and also the purpose of accumulating
social capital. The impact of such is that the control over working hours is
the biggest trouble that both politicians and employers can face. The major
precondition for control in this case does become to have the working and
leisure hours separated, and also to separate working days from the holidays.
Self-efficacy and
performance
An individual’s theory of intelligence is considered to be
the belief that the person has about nature and their working intellect. The
researches related to these theories have grown out of the study of individual
achievement goals. It was discovered in this research; the ones related to
studies, that the some students were highly oriented about validating their
ability (one that makes them to pursue performance goals), while others were
found to be oriented towards learning in the same situation (they purse mastery
and learning goals). The impact of such is that it raised the question of: what
are the factors that determine the goals favoured by students? It is affirmed
in social-cognitive theory that self-efficacy belief are born out of a broader
framework of “self-theories” as conceptualized by Dweck
(1999), which cover motivation and performance. In these theories,
investigation were conducted in relation to how people develop themselves and
their believes (which is their self-esteem) and how they make use of these
beliefs to create psychological world, feelings, behaviours, and thoughts that
shape them into whom they become. In this approach, revelations are made about
why some people are motivated to work harder, while others turnout to become
helpless in some situation and as such, self-defeating. The conclusion of this
study is an exploration of the implication of the concept of self-esteem, one
that suggest rethinking it in relation to its role in motivation, as well as
the factors that foster them. In one of the two implicit theories, “entity
theory of intelligence”, it was discovered that when individuals subscribe to
this theory are less likely to engage themselves in challenging tasks and the
impact is that they are at risk of underperforming based on the fact that they
conceive intelligence to be stable and fixed. Such people seem to have a higher
desire at proving themselves to others, one that would help them to be seen as
smart and avert being seen as unintelligent but at the end of the day, they
seem to prefer not putting in the expected effort. In contrast, there is the
second implicit theory, which is the “incremental theory” and it postulates
that intelligence is fluid, malleable and changeable. In the case that people
subscribe to this theory, they are content with the satisfaction that comes
from the learning process and they might highlight opportunities for
improvement as well as put in necessary efforts to work hard and provider
enhanced performance outcomes. In any case, it is important to point out that
they don’t focus on the outcome attainable from participating in the learning
process. Self-efficacy is elucidated by the implicit theories of intelligence,
and other elucidations include their level of self-regulation, pursued goals,
and achievements. In such circumstances, both achievement (Woolfolk Hoy & Burke-Spero, 2005) and
motivation of the person (Valentine, DuBois, &
Cooper, 2004) could potentially become influenced. A good example is in
the case where the person employ the entity intelligence that does favour
mastery goals (Fives, Hamman, & Olivarez, 2005),
it shouldn’t be expected that they can mobilize cognitive and meta-cognitive
measures such as planning, elaborating and monitoring (Ommundsen,
Haugen, & Thorleif, 2005), and as such, it is difficult for them to
thrive when faced with difficult situations (Busato,
Prins, Elshout, & Hamaker, 2000) as well as feel threatened by the
demand for them to do more (Ames, 1992). On
the same note, when the person is oriented more towards the incremental
morality, they would probably be more positive, seek mastery as against
performance, put in more efforts in their work process and be more self-regulated
(Zhang, 2003). It has been discovered that
those who believe in incremental intelligence do show higher level of
self-efficacy, and they are better motivated as well as focused on the real of
meta-cognition as well as more likely to put in more efforts in their job and
take challenging tasks (Komarraju, Karau, &
Schmeck, 2009).
While there are indications that self-regulation is related
to the believe of self-efficacy (Wolfe &
Johnson, 1995; Tuckman, 2003), the relationship of self-efficacy seem to
be inconsistent when measured with goals, motivation and achievement. Thus,
there are suggestions that when motivation, thoughts, affect, and achievement
are put into consideration, a strong pursuit of performance goals and mastery
might become futile, where it has also been shown that the mastery goals would
have upper hand (Dörnyei & Ushioda, 2010).
In the case that the person does feel that it is important to coordinate their
motivation and persistence in line with their tasks at hand; it does seem that
they will tailor the strategies they prefer in line with the created
conditions. For instance, when they are handling a task that considers it
necessary to implemented performance strategies, they might resort to
performance goals. When people encounter difficult materials, it might justify
their utilization of information processing policies. The extrinsic measures
might be sought in the event that they desire to stay focused on unrelated
materials (Komarraju & Karau, 2005). The
relationship between achievement and self-efficacy is linked to factors such as
implicit theories of intelligence, self-regulation and achievement goals in
their complex nature.
Bandura ( 2001) while
delineating self-efficacy within the social-cognitive theory framework did
describe self-efficacy to be a motivation orientation that does stimulate the
grit when people are faced with difficult situation, bringing about enhancement
on their deliberate actions, encouraging long-term views, fostering
self-regulation and making it possible for people to self-correct whenever such
is considered necessary. In a number of researches, self-efficacy has been
presented as a reliable predictor of performance and motivation, with it being
considered as a factor that doesn’t alter in line with time, different
communities and environment (De Raad &
Shouwenburg, 1996; Duckworth, Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007; Farsides
& Woodfield, 2003). Abouserie (1995)
conducted another study where it was marinated that involvement with success or
failure could be related to a strong or weak level of self-efficacy and such
relationship could be used to determine the performance of people. There is
also another claim in literatures that the motivational module of self-efficacy
could lead to an apparent inducement of the academic performance of students (Ashwin, 2006; Pritchard & Wilson, 2003; Ridgell &
Lounsbury, 2004; Chamorro-Premusic & Furnham, 2003). In another
report, it was made known by Miller & Brickman
(2004) that strong performance might be associated with increase in
one’s confidence and power and such would stimulate the individuals into taking
higher level of responsibility for successful completion of tasks and projects.
Based on this view, it was also noted by Frey &
Determan (2004) that people who have the superior ability needed for
work do show higher level of performance and attain superior evaluation. It is
apparent that such individuals have higher level of self-efficacy and less
anxiety. Furthering on this, similar fluctuations were underlined by Livengood (1992) within a single semester in the
course of which the students were offered continuous feedback on their
performance. While it is reported that the high performing students that they
are highly self-confident and attributed their high performance to greater
value for their learning, the low achieving students were found to be less in
confidence. Therefore, it was concluded that when vigorously predicting
academic performance, one need to combine self-efficacy and motivation, and as
a result of its complexity, such relationship is said to demand higher
exploration.
How creative people
can utilize self-efficacy to enhance performance when faced with anxiety and
task management issues: it is all about self-regulation
When faced with anxiety and task management issues, the
creative individuals are said to be able to enhance their performance with the
aid of self-efficacy through self-regulation. In self-regulated learning, the
emphasis is on autonomy and control by the individual that directs, monitors,
and regulates their actions towards the aim of acquiring information, expanding
their expertise, and self-improvement. In particular, the self-regulated
learners are said to be cognizant of their strength and weaknesses, and they
have different kinds of strategies which they apply in their right manner to
handle their daily challenging tasks. These sets of people are of the belief
that intelligence is malleable (one that is opposed to the fixed and entity
view of intelligence) and as such, they have their success or failure
attributed to the factors that are under their control. It is also believed by
these people that opportunities to partake in challenging tasks, practice their
learning, gain higher level understanding of the task, and put in the right
effort that would ensure that they create desired academic success. To some
extent, these features might help when it comes to explaining why the
self-regulated learner will normally show a high sense of self-efficacy. For
the students that show such, the features of such success will likely go beyond
their academic life into real world setting (Grzegorek,
Slaney, Franze, & Rice, 2004). Insight was offered by Ozer & Benet-Martinez (2005) on the
relationship between self-efficacy and performance. The claim that this is
based on entails motivational components, cognitive and meta-cognitive features
as well as resource management. In other words, it is believed that people who
have high self-efficacy seem to be more tenacious, hard working, have preference
for dealing with challenging tasks, and they are better positioned to cope with
their anxiety. In accordance with Snyder (2000),
it is marinated that such people are more likely to resort to self-regulating
measures like goal setting, self-evaluation, and self-monitoring. On the same
note, it was pointed out by Entwistle &
Entwistle (1970) that those with self-efficacy have higher tendency to
show greater self-control, uphold mastery goals, and put in more efforts when
faced with difficult situations with then eventual outcome being enhanced
performance. The work of Zhang (2002) also
offered insight in this, were it was contended that people who have higher
self-efficacy can perform at higher level
due to their ability to cope with difficulty more effectively, and in
clarification, the elements of master goal orientation might be added based on
the work of Anderson, Boyles, & Rainie (2012).
It is also possible that such people considered their task to be interesting,
valuated, and related to their objectives, which employ them to put in more
pertinent efforts (Duckworth & Seligman, 2005).
Thus, the summary is that self-efficacy can be used to bring about enhanced
performance be it in the form of goal-setting or effort regulation, and both of
these are intrinsic motivation (Payne, Youngcourt,
& Beaubien, 2007; Pajares, 1996; Doyle & Moen, 1978).
Therefore, creative people can enhance their performance
with self-efficacy when faced with anxiety and task management issues as
pointed out by Laurillard (2002), that they
can do so by enhancing their self-regulation and coordinating the circumstances
(internal and external) that surrounds them. In the hallmark of this measure is
that under such circumstances, the self-efficacious individuals are aware of
whom to ask for help, capable of making decisions in relation to their required
efforts, and have their intent designed attain goals as well as schedule their
time righteously. That is to say, the self-efficacious individuals are better
positioned in cases of anxiety and time management issues to counter their
challenges and produce performance as normal or even higher in line with the
view of Green, Nelson & Marsh (2006).
These sets of people are courageous and even under such extreme conditions,
they still rely strongly on their will-power and input necessary remedies to
ensure that these issues doesn’t come against their desire to succeed,
effectively leading to better performance in the end. Therefore, when faced
with anxiety and time management issues, creative people can utilize
self-efficacy to enhance their performance by remaining focused and courageous.
Conclusion
Self-efficacy, as defined in this work, is the strong belief
that people have about their ability and potentials. This pushes them to do
more and produce more, strongly believing that they are capable of doing
anything irrespective of how challenging it might be. As such, when these
people are faced with anxiety and time management issues, they are able to
sustain or even enhance their performance by remaining focused and being
strategic in the way they go about such task.
References
Abouserie, R.
(1995). Self-esteem and achievement motivation as determinants of students'
approaches to studying . Studies in higher education , 20, 19-26.
Aguinis, H., Boyd,
B. K., Pierce, C. A., & Short, J. C.(2011). Walking new avenues in
management research methods and theories: Bridging micro and macro domains.
Journal of Management, 37, 395–403.
Alberti, L.B.(1971).The
Albertis of Florence: Leon Battista Alberti’s Della Famiglia. Lewisburg, PA:
Bucknell University Press. (Original work published 1444.)
Amabile, T.
M. (1982), Social psychology of creativity: a consensual assessment technique,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43, pp. 997-1013.
Amabile, T.
M. (1983), Social psychology of creativity: A componential conceptualization,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, pp. 357-376.
Amabile, T.
M. (1996), Creativity in context, Boulder, CO: Westview.
Amabile, T.
M. (1998), How to kill creativity, Harvard Business Review 76 (5), pp. 76-86.
Ames, C.
(1992). Classrooms, goals, structures and student motivation. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 84, 267-271.
Anderson, A.,
Boyles, J., & Rainie, L. (2012). The future of higher education . Retrieved
2019, from Pew Internet Research Center:
http://www.pewinternet.org/topics/future-of-the-internet.aspx
Anderson, N.,
Potocnik, K., Zhou, J. (2014), Innovation and creativity in organizations: A
state-of-the-science review, prospective commentary, and guiding framework,
Journal of Management 40 (5), pp. 1297-1333.
Arndt, J.,
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S. et al. (1999), Creativity and terror management:
Evidence that creative activity increases guilt and social projection following
mortality salience, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77 (1), pp.
19-32
Ashwin, P.
(2006). Changing higher education. Hoboken: Routledge.
Bandura, A.
(2001). Social cognitive theory: an agentive perspective . Annual review of
psychology, 52, 1-26.
Barevičiūtė,
J. (2014), Pagrindiniai kūrybiškumo ir kūrybingumo aspektai šiuolaikiniuose
humanitariniuose bei socialiniuose moksluose (The aspects of creativity and
creativeness in contemporary humanities and social sciences), Filosofija.
Sociologija 25(1), pp. 19-28
Barling, J.,
Cheung, D., & Kelloway, E. K. (1996). Time management and achievement
striving interact to predict car sales performance. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 81, 821–826.
Beghetto, R.
A., and Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.)
(2010). Nurturing creativity in the
classroom. UK: Cambridge University Press
Bilton, C.,
and Cummings, S. (2014), A framework for creative management and managing
creativity, In: C. Bilton, S. Cummings (eds.), Handbook of management and
creativity, Regency: Edward Elgar Publishing.
Brown, A.,
Bransford, J., Ferrara, R., & Campione, J. (1983). Learning and remembering
and understanding. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3,
pp. 515-629). New York: Wiley.
Busato, V.,
Prins, F., Elshout, J., & Hamaker, C. (2000). Intellectual ability,
learning style, personality, achievement motivation and academic success of
psychology students in higher education . Personality and individual
differences , 29, 1057-1068.
Byron, K.,
& Khazanchi, S. (2011). A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship
of state and trait anxiety to performance on figural and verbal creative tasks.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 269–283.
doi:10.1177/0146167210392788
Carlsson, I.,
Wendt, P. E., & Risberg, J. (2000). On the neurobiology of creativity.
Differences in frontal activity between high and low creative subjects.
Neuropsychologia, 38, 873–885. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00128-1
Černevičiūtė,
J. (2014), Creativity Understandings, Evolution: from Genius to Creative
Systems, Santalka: Filosofija, Komunikacija 22 (2), pp. 113-125.
Chamorro-Premusic,
T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality predicts academic performance:
evidence from two longitidinal samples. Journal of reserach in personality, 37,
319-338
Chen, M.-H.,
Chang, Y.-Y., and Lo, Y.-H. (2015), Creativity cognitive style, conflict, and
career success for creative entrepreneurs, Journal of Business Research 68 (4),
pp. 906-910.
Choi, J. N.,
& Moran, S. V. (2009). Why not procrastinate? Development and validation of
a new active procrastination scale. The Journal of Social Psychology, 149,
195–211.
Chu, A. H.
C., & Choi, J. N. (2005). Rethinking procrastination: Positive effects of
Bactive^ procrastination behavior on attitudes and performance. The Journal of
Social Psychology, 145, 245–264.
Claessens, B.
C., van Eerde, W., Rutte, C. G., & Roe, R. A. (2007). A review of the time
management literature. Personnel Review, 36, 255 –276.
De Dreu, C.
K. W., Nijstad, B. A., & Baas, M. (2011). Behavioral activation links to
creativity because of increased cognitive flexibility. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 2, 72–80. doi:10.1177/1948550610381789
De Raad, B.,
& Shouwenburg, H. (1996). Personality in Learning and education . European
journal of personality, 10, 303-336
Dörnyei, Z.,
& Ushioda, E. (2010). Teaching and researching motivation (2 ed.). London:
Longman.
Doyle, K.,
& Moen, R. (1978). Toward the definition of the domain of academic
motivation. Journal of educational psychology, 70(2), 231-236.
Drazin, R.,
Glynn, M. A., Kazanjian, R. K. (1999), Multilevel theorizing about creativity
in organizations: A sense making perspective, Academy of Management Review 24,
pp. 286-307.
Duckworth,
A., Peterson, C., Matthews, M., & Kelly, D. (2007). Grit: perseverance and
passion for long term goals . Journal of personality and social psychology, 92,
1087-1101.
Dweck, C.
(1999). Self-theories: their role in motivation, personality and development.
Philadelphia , PA: The psychology press.
Dweck, C.
(2006). Mindset. New York: Random House.
Entwistle,
N., & Entwistle, D. (1970). The relationships between personality, study
methods, and academic performance . British journal of educational psychology,
40, 132-143.
Farsides, T.,
& Woodfield, R. (2003). Individual differences and undergraduate academic
success: the roles of personality, intelligence and application . Personality
and individual differences , 40, 1225-1243.
Fiori, L. M.,
Wanner, B., Jomphe, V., Croteau, J., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., Bureau, A.,
Turecki, G. (2010). Association of Polyaminergic Loci With Anxiety, Mood
Disorders, and Attempted Suicide. Polyamine Variants in Psychiatric Disorders.
5(11). 1-9.
Fives, H.,
Hamman, D., & Olivarez, A. (2005). Does Burnout start with student
teaching: analyzing efficacy, burnout, and support during the student-teaching
semester . Montreal, CA: Annual meeting of the America Educational Research
Association.
Florida, R.
(2002), The Rise of the Creative Class, New York: Basic Books.
Ford, C. M.,
Gioia, D. A. (2000), Factors influencing creativity in the domain of managerial
decision making, Journal of Management 26 (4), pp. 705-732.
Frey, M.,
& Determan, D. (2004). Scholastic assessment or g? The relationship between
scholastic assessment test and general cognitive ability. Psychological
science, 15, 373-378.
Gardner, H.
(1983). Frames of mind: the theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic
Books.
Granqvist, N.,
&Gustafsson, R.(2016).Temporal institutional work. Academy of Management Journal,
59, 1009–1035.
Green, J.,
Nelson, G., A.J., M., & Marsh, H. (2006). The causal ordering of
self-concept and academic motivation and its effect on academic achievement.
International education journal, 7, 534-546.
Grzegorek,
J., Slaney, R., Franze, S., & Rice, K. (2004). Self-criticism, dependency,
self-esteem, and grade point stisfaction among clusters of perfectionists and
non-perfectionists. Journal of counseling psychology, 51, 192- 200
Henderson,
P., Rosen, D., & Mascaro, N. (2007). Empirical Study on the Healing Nature
of Mandalas. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1(3), 148-154.
Hill, W.
(2002). Learning: a survey of psychological interpretations (7 ed.). Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.
Johnsen, C.
G. (2015), Deconstructing the future of management: Pharmakon, Gary Hamel and
the impossibility of invention, Futures, 68 (SI), pp. 57-66.
Khouzam, H.
R. (2009). "Anxiety Disorders: Guidelines for Effective Primary Care. Part
1: Diagnosis". Consultant 49 (3). http://www.consultantlive.com/display/article/10162/1387611
Kim, E.,
& Seo, E. H. (2013). The relationship of flow and self-regulated learning
to active procrastination. Social Behavior and Personality: An International
Journal, 41, 1099–1113.
King, L. A.,
Walker, L. M., & Broyles, S. J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor
model. Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189–203. doi:10.1006/
jrpe.1996.0013
Klein,
Melanie. (1975). Infantile anxiety situations reflected in a work of art and in
the creative impulse. In The Writings
of Melanie Klein (Vol. 1). London: Hogarth. (Reprinted from International
Journal of PsychoAnalysis, 10, (1929) 436-443.
Klimczuk, A.
(2014), Barriers to the development of creative industries in culturally
diverse region, Santalka: Filosofija, Komunikacija 22(2), pp. 145-152.
Koch, C. J.,
& Kleinmann, M.(2002).A stitch in time saves nine: Behavioural
decision-making explanations for time management problems. European Journal of
Work and Organizational Psychology, 11, 199–217.
Komarraju,
M., Karau, S., & Schmeck, R. (2009). Role of the big five personality
traits in predicting college students' academic motivation and achievement.
Learning and individual differences , 19, 47-52.
Lane, C., and
Lup, D. (2015), Cooking under fire: Managing multilevel tensions between
creativity and innovation in haute cuisine, Industry and Innovation, 22 (8),
pp. 654-676.
Laurillard,
D. (2002). Rethinking university teaching:a framework for effective use of
educational technology (2nd edition ed.). London: Routledge Plamer.
Lay, C. H.,
& Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait procrastination, time management, and
academic behavior. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 8, 647–662.
Livengood, J.
(1992). Students' motivational goals and beliefs about effort and ability as
they relate to college academic success. Research in Higher education, 33,
247-261.
Martindale,
C., Anderson, K., Moore, K., & West, A. N. (1996). Creativity,
oversensitivity, and rate of habituation. Personality and Individual
Differences, 20, 423–427. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(95)00193-X
Martinez-pons,
M. (2002). A social cognitiveview of parental influence on student academic
self-regulation. Theory into practice, 61, 126-131.
McCombs, B.,
& Marzano, R. (1990). Putting the self in self-regulated learning: the self
as agent in integrating skill and will. Educational psychologist, 25, 51-70.
McLeod, P.
L., Baron, R. S., Marti, M. W., Yoon, K. (1997), The eyes have it: Minority
influence in face-to-face and computer-mediated group discussions, Journal of
Applied Psychology 82, pp. 706-718.
Metcalfe, J.,
& Shimamura, A. (1994). Metacognition: knowledge about knowing. MA: MIT
Press.
Miller, R.,
& Brickman, S. (2004). A model ofo future-oriented motivation and
self-regulation. Educational Psychology Review, 16, 9-33
Mitchell, L.
A., MacDonald, R. A. R., & Knussen, C. (2008). An Investigation of the
Effects of Music and Art on Pain Perception. Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts, 2(3), 162-170.
Morris, P.
(1990). Metacognition. In M. Eysenck (Ed.), The blackwell dictionary of
cognitive psychology (pp. 225-229). Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Murphy, P.,
& Alexander, P. (2000). A motivated Exploration of Motivation Terminalogy.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 3-53.
Ollendick,
T., Dailey, D., & Shapiro, E. (1983). Vicarious reinforcement: expected and
unexpected effects. Journal of applied behavior analysis, 16, 483-491.
Ommundsen,
Y., Haugen, R., & Thorleif, L. (2005). Academic self-concept, implicit
theories of ability and self-regulation strategies. Scandinavian journal of
educational research, 49, 461-474.
Ozer, D.,
& Benet-Martinez, V. (2005). Personality and the prediction of
consequential outcomes. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 401-421.
Pajares, F.
(1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational
Research, 66, 543-578.
Park, S. W.,
& Sperling, R. (2012). Academic procrastinators and their self-regulation.
Psychology, 3, 12 –23.
Passer, M.
W., Smith, R., Holt, N., Bremner, A.,
Sutherland, E. and Vliek, M. (2009). McGrath Hill Higher Education; UK: McGrath
Hill companies Inc.
Payne, S.,
Youngcourt, S., & Beaubien, J. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the
goal-orientation nomological net . Journal of applied psychology, 92, 128-150.
Pečiulis, Ž.
(2015), Vienetiškumas ir tiražas – kūrybos visuomenės paradoksas (Paradoxes of
the creative society), Filosofija. Sociologija 26(1), pp. 81-85.
Potur, A. A.,
& Barkul, Ö. (2010). Gender and creative thinking in education: A
theoretical and experimental overview. ITU A|Z, 6(2), 44-57
Pritchard,
M., & Wilson, G. (2003). Using emotional and social factors to predict
student success. Journal of college student development, 44, 18-28.
Pruskus, V.
(2015), Kūrybingumo panaudojimo gerinant aplinkos kokybę etiniai ir socialiniai
kultūriniai aspektai (Ethical and sociocultural aspects of creativity use in
improving the quality of the environment), Filosofija. Sociologija 26(3), pp.
201-209.
Rank, O.
(1993). The trauma of birth. Reprinted, with a new introduction by E. James
Lieberman. New York: Dover Press, 1993.
Reimeris, R.
(2016), Theoretical features of the creative society, Creativity studies 9(1),
pp. 15-24.
Ridgell, S.,
& Lounsbury, J. (2004). Prediciting academic success:general intelligence,
"big five" personality traits and work drive. colleg student journal
, 38, 607-618.
Sadeghi, A.,
Basirani, N., Asadi Bidmeshki, E., Panahi Mirshekar, A., Amirshahi, M., and
Salehin S. (2010). Prevalence of anxiety and its relationship with self-esteem
among Zabol University students, Iran. Educational Research, 1(5), 140-144.
Sadock, B.
J., Sadock V. A., (2007). Kaplan & Sadock’s Synopsis of Psychiatry.
Publisher: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
Schraw, G.,
Wadkins, T., & Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: A grounded
theory of academic procrastination. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 12
–25.
Schunk, D.
(2004). Learning theories: an educational perspective (4 ed.). Columbu, OH:
Merrill/Prentice-Hall.
Seneca, L. A.
(2014). Hardship and happiness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original
work published 50.)
Silvia, P. J.
and Kimbrel, N. A. (2010). A Dimensional Analysis of Creativity and Mental
Illness: Do Anxiety and Depression Symptoms Predict Creative Cognition,
Creative Accomplishments, and Creative SelfConcepts? Psychology of Aesthetics,
Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 2-10
Snyder, R.
(2000). The relationship between learning styles and multiple intelligences and
academic achievement of high school students. High school journal, 83, 11-20.
Steel, P.
(2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review
of quintessential selfregulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 65 –94.
Steel, P.
(2010). Arousal, avoidant and decisional procrastinators: Do they exist?
Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 926 –934.
Sutton, R. I.
(2001), The weird rules of creativity, Harvard Business Review 79 (8), pp. 94-
103.
Tuckman, B.
(2003). The effect of learning and motivation strategies training on college
students' achievement. Journal of college student development, 44, 430-437.
Valentine,
J., DuBois, D., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relations between self-beliefs and
academic achievement- a systematic review . Educational psychologist, 39,
111-133.
Winnicott,
D., (1999). Playing and reality. London: Routledge.
Wolfe, R.,
& Johnson, S. (1995). Personality as a predictor of college performance.
Educational and psychological styles in addition to personality traits? .
Personality and educational differences, 33, 445-458.
Woolfolk Hoy,
A., & Burke-Spero, R. (2005). Changes in teacher efficacy during the early
years of teaching: a comparison of four measures. teaching and teacher
education, 21, 343-346.
Zhang, L.
(2002). Measuring thinking styles in addition to personality traits?
Personality and individual differences, 33, 445-458.
Zhang, L.
(2003). Does the big five predict learning approaches. Personality and individual
differences, 34, 1431-1446.
great
ReplyDeletethank you
ReplyDelete